Researchers say pretend play should be studied in children from a younger age
A new study exploring the origins of pretend play suggests infants perform interactional patterns with elements of pretense a lot earlier than thought.
Pretend play is often considered a developmental landmark, being linked to emotion regulation, language skills, cognitive reasoning, and problem-solving. It is widely accepted a child begins participating in make-believe activities when they have developed the capacity to recognize they are doing it and, in most cases, studies focus on infants who are somewhat verbal.
However, new research by the University of Portsmouth and Lund University, Sweden, has drawn connections between pretense and a child’s early-on playful interactions, such as clowning and teasing.
Iris Nomikou, from the University of Portsmouth’s Department of Psychology, said, “We often see children using props that stand for something else, like a stick as a sword or a bowl as a hat. But before they become directors of imaginary worlds, they start off small by being actors and performing something unconventional.
“This can be pulling funny facial expressions, making unnatural noises, and even infant acting—like they’re going to do something when they’re not—to get the attention of a parent, caregiver or friend.”
Pretend play is usually defined as an activity with a symbolic character, in which a signifier (e.g., a banana) is used to represent the meaning of a signified (e.g., a telephone). But the paper (to be published in the journal Applied Psycholinguistics and available now as a PsyArXiv preprint) says variations exist in its quality, emergence and developmental progress across different contexts and cultures.
The authors argue that pretense should stop being defined as an end-product of cognitive development, and instead an interpersonal one. Its origins can then be moved to much earlier in infancy than was originally thought possible.
Valentina Fantasia, from the Department of Philosophy and Cognitive Science at Lund University, added, “There is observational evidence of a child as young as eight months old pretending to give an object to someone, then withdrawing it as soon as they reach out.
“Most parents and caregivers have experienced these types of interactions, but still not much attention has been dedicated to investigating their broadest developmental impact or the continuity that exists with pretend play.
“What these earlier and later forms of actions have in common most, is that they are spaces in which infants and children can construct and explore different kinds of realities with meaningful others.”
Source: Read Full Article